meta:paragraph-count: 124 custom:ScaleCrop: false extended-properties:Template: Normal.dotm Author: Victoria custom:Mendeley Citation Style_1: http://www.zotero.org/styles/apa Last-Modified: 2021-02-16T19:59:33Z meta:last-author: Charlotte GW xmpTPg:NPages: 19 custom:HyperlinksChanged: false dc:publisher: BBK custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_SAU9bhBkts_37: he published and unpublished findings in implicit Theory of Mind research.", "ISSN": "23523409"}, "uris": ["http://www.mendeley.com/documents/?uuid=d5e1a22c-bc5e-37bc-a8a3-172144ccc302"]}]} custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_SAU9bhBkts_36: ch paradigm, how many replications, partial replications and non-replications were identified and how many of them were published or unpublished. Furthermore, descriptive data and sample sizes are reported. The dataset provides a qualitative overview of t custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_SAU9bhBkts_35: }], "title": "Implicit Theory of Mind \u2013 An overview of current replications and non-replications", "abstract": "The current dataset contains a qualitative summary of (non-)replication studies of implicit Theory of Mind paradigms. It summarizes for ea custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_SAU9bhBkts_34: ffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Kulke", "given": "Louisa", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Rakoczy", "given": "Hannes", "parse-names": false custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_SAU9bhBkts_33: 1794558b-9963-3bef-aed9-b39d95a549f4"]}, {"id": "ITEM-4", "itemData": {"issued": {"date-parts": [["2018"]]}, "type": "article-journal", "volume": "16", "DOI": "10.1016/j.dib.2017.11.016", "container-title": "Data in Brief", "id": "ITEM-4", "author": [{"su custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_SAU9bhBkts_32: that they reflect the veridical abilities of 18-month-old infants, who may track others' knowledge and ignorance but may not consistently represent the contents of others' beliefs.", "ISSN": "08852014"}, "uris": ["http://www.mendeley.com/documents/?uuid= custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_SAU9bhBkts_31: the previously reported violation of expectation results on which we were attempting to build. However, we also discuss the consistency of our data with other findings and hypotheses regarding early-developing theory of mind, and consider the possibility custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_SAU9bhBkts_30: ent will engage in on the basis of false beliefs. Instead, looking times were consistent with infants having no clear prediction about the agent's actions under conditions of false belief. We discuss factors that might account for our failure to reproduce custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_YfcrMlUt49_5: "", "family": "Kov\u00e1cs", "given": "\u00c1gnes M.", "parse-names": false}], "PMID": "30322932", "id": "ITEM-1", "ISSN": "0027-8424", "DOI": "10.1073/pnas.1803505115", "type": "article-journal", "issue": "45", "ISBN": "0027-8424", "container-title": "Pr custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_YfcrMlUt49_4: : "Ol\u00e1h", "given": "Katalin", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Csibra", "given": "Gergely", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": language: en-US custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_YfcrMlUt49_7: te and track others\u2019 beliefs. Beliefs can be attributed in at least two different ways: prospectively, during the observation of belief-inducing situations, and in a retrospective manner, based on episodic retrieval of the details of the events that custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_YfcrMlUt49_6: oceedings of the National Academy of Sciences", "title": "Retrospective attribution of false beliefs in 3-year-old children", "abstract": "A current debate in psychology and cognitive science concerns the nature of young children\u2019s ability to attribu custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_YfcrMlUt49_9: nt event, which was witnessed by a person wearing sunglasses (Experiment 1). Having later discovered that the sunglasses were opaque, 36-month-olds correctly inferred that the person must have formed a false belief about the location of the objects and us custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_YfcrMlUt49_8: brought about the beliefs. We developed a task in which only retrospective attribution, but not prospective belief tracking, would allow children to correctly infer that someone had a false belief. Eighteen- and 36-month-old children observed a displaceme custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_CsuMkuND1e_26: s aimed at building on past research to better understand infants' theory of mind capacities and their relationship to preschoolers' explicit theory of mind. One task fully replicated evidence of 2-year-old children's sensitivity to others' ignorance. The custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_CsuMkuND1e_25: th varying representational demands", "abstract": "We attempted to reproduce three tests of theory of mind in infants using implicit tasks that have been previously reported in the literature. These efforts were intended as initial steps in larger project custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_CsuMkuND1e_24: : "", "family": "Carey", "given": "Susan", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Saxe", "given": "Rebecca", "parse-names": false}], "title": "Replications of implicit theory of mind tasks wi custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_CsuMkuND1e_23: ly": "Hobbs", "given": "Kathryn", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Bardis", "given": "Alexandros", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle" custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_YfcrMlUt49_1: ADDIN CSL_CITATION {"schema": "https://github.com/citation-style-language/schema/raw/master/csl-citation.json", "properties": {"noteIndex": 0}, "mendeley": {"manualFormatting": "Kir\u00e1ly, Ol\u00e1h, Csibra, & Kov\u00e1cs, 2018)", "formattedCitation": " custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_CsuMkuND1e_29: ng times were consistent with infants having no clear prediction about the agent's actions under conditions of false belief. We discuss factors that might account for our failure to reproduce the previously reported violation of expectation results on whi custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_CsuMkuND1e_28: n theory of mind capacities. In contrast, our violation of expectation tasks failed to reproduce evidence that, by 18 months of age, infants form specific expectations about the actions an agent will engage in on the basis of false beliefs. Instead, looki custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_YfcrMlUt49_3: date-parts": [["2018"]]}, "author": [{"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Kir\u00e1ly", "given": "Ildik\u00f3", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family" custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_CsuMkuND1e_27: results of another task testing for similar capacities in 18-month-old infants also elicited behavior similar to the original findings, although in that case we only conducted one of two conditions critical for demonstrating that performance depended upo custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_YfcrMlUt49_2: (Kir\u00e1ly et al., 2018)", "plainTextFormattedCitation": "(Kir\u00e1ly et al., 2018)", "previouslyFormattedCitation": "(Kir\u00e1ly, Ol\u00e1h, Csibra, & Kov\u00e1cs, 2018)"}, "citationItems": [{"id": "ITEM-1", "itemData": {"volume": "115", "issued": {" custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_CsuMkuND1e_22: tive Development", "id": "ITEM-3", "author": [{"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Powell", "given": "Lindsey J.", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "fami custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_CsuMkuND1e_21: ["http://www.mendeley.com/documents/?uuid=39ffb97a-51ed-321c-8ec0-555a9204b896"]}, {"id": "ITEM-3", "itemData": {"issued": {"date-parts": [["2018"]]}, "type": "article", "DOI": "10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.10.004", "ISBN": "0885-2014", "container-title": "Cogni custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_CsuMkuND1e_20: robustness and validity of existing implicit ToM tasks needs to be treated with more caution than previously practiced, and that not all non-verbal tasks and measures are equally suited to tap into implicit ToM processing.", "ISSN": "08852014"}, "uris": custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_CsuMkuND1e_15: validity of four non-verbal measures", "abstract": "A growing body of infant studies with various implicit, non-verbal measures has suggested that Theory of Mind (ToM) may emerge much earlier than previously assumed. While explicit verbal ToM findings are custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_CsuMkuND1e_14: en": "Hannes", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Liszkowski", "given": "Ulf", "parse-names": false}], "title": "How (not) to measure infant Theory of Mind: Testing the replicability and custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_CsuMkuND1e_13: EM-2", "author": [{"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "D\u00f6rrenberg", "given": "Sebastian", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Rakoczy", "giv custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_kok2ZwgpW1_9: nt event, which was witnessed by a person wearing sunglasses (Experiment 1). Having later discovered that the sunglasses were opaque, 36-month-olds correctly inferred that the person must have formed a false belief about the location of the objects and us custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_CsuMkuND1e_12: eley.com/documents/?uuid=3cb02e59-0d54-3a45-8b72-a18b3f6b3825"]}, {"id": "ITEM-2", "itemData": {"issued": {"date-parts": [["2018"]]}, "type": "article-newspaper", "DOI": "10.1016/j.cogdev.2018.01.001", "container-title": "Cognitive Development", "id": "IT custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_kok2ZwgpW1_8: brought about the beliefs. We developed a task in which only retrospective attribution, but not prospective belief tracking, would allow children to correctly infer that someone had a false belief. Eighteen- and 36-month-old children observed a displaceme custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_CsuMkuND1e_19: utcomes, indicating limited evidence for implicit ToM processes under certain conditions. There were no systematic correlations of false belief processing between the tasks, thus failing to provide convergent validity. The present results suggest that the custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_CsuMkuND1e_18: on-based tasks did not replicate previous findings, suggesting that these tasks do not reliably measure ToM. Looking time and new pupil dilation measures revealed sensitivity to belief-incongruent outcomes which interacted with the presentation order of o custom:AppVersion: 16.0000 custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_CsuMkuND1e_17: cit ToM tasks using four different measures with 24-month-olds (N = 66): Anticipatory looking, looking times and pupil dilation in violation-of-expectation paradigms, and spontaneous communicative interaction. Results of anticipatory looking and interacti custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_CsuMkuND1e_16: highly replicable and show convergent validity, systematic replication studies of infant ToM, as well as convergent validations of these measures, are still missing. Here, we report a systematic study of the replicability and convergent validity of impli custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_CsuMkuND1e_11: rather than a previous location. These findings point towards the fragility of early belief-related action anticipation before the age of 4 years, when children start passing traditional false belief tasks.", "ISSN": "08852014"}, "uris": ["http://www.mend custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_CsuMkuND1e_10: e between the ages of 3 and 4 years. Children anticipated correctly only by the age of 4 years, and performed at chance at the ages of 2 and 3 years. Moreover, we found correct anticipation only when the agent falsely believed an object to be in its last cp:revision: 56 custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_kok2ZwgpW1_3: date-parts": [["2018"]]}, "author": [{"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Kir\u00e1ly", "given": "Ildik\u00f3", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family" custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_kok2ZwgpW1_2: (Kir\u00e1ly et al., 2018)", "plainTextFormattedCitation": "(Kir\u00e1ly et al., 2018)", "previouslyFormattedCitation": "(Kir\u00e1ly, Ol\u00e1h, Csibra, & Kov\u00e1cs, 2018)"}, "citationItems": [{"id": "ITEM-1", "itemData": {"volume": "115", "issued": {" dcterms:created: 2019-10-07T08:31:00Z custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_kok2ZwgpW1_1: ADDIN CSL_CITATION {"schema": "https://github.com/citation-style-language/schema/raw/master/csl-citation.json", "properties": {"noteIndex": 0}, "mendeley": {"manualFormatting": "Kir\u00e1ly, Ol\u00e1h, Csibra, & Kov\u00e1cs, 2018)", "formattedCitation": " dcterms:modified: 2021-02-16T19:59:33Z custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_kok2ZwgpW1_7: te and track others\u2019 beliefs. Beliefs can be attributed in at least two different ways: prospectively, during the observation of belief-inducing situations, and in a retrospective manner, based on episodic retrieval of the details of the events that custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_kok2ZwgpW1_6: oceedings of the National Academy of Sciences", "title": "Retrospective attribution of false beliefs in 3-year-old children", "abstract": "A current debate in psychology and cognitive science concerns the nature of young children\u2019s ability to attribu custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_kok2ZwgpW1_5: "", "family": "Kov\u00e1cs", "given": "\u00c1gnes M.", "parse-names": false}], "PMID": "30322932", "id": "ITEM-1", "ISSN": "0027-8424", "DOI": "10.1073/pnas.1803505115", "type": "article-journal", "issue": "45", "ISBN": "0027-8424", "container-title": "Pr custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_kok2ZwgpW1_4: : "Ol\u00e1h", "given": "Katalin", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Csibra", "given": "Gergely", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": extended-properties:TotalTime: 658 Content-Type: application/vnd.openxmlformats-officedocument.wordprocessingml.document custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_CsuMkuND1e_36: research.", "ISSN": "23523409"}, "uris": ["http://www.mendeley.com/documents/?uuid=d5e1a22c-bc5e-37bc-a8a3-172144ccc302"]}]} custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_CsuMkuND1e_35: -replications were identified and how many of them were published or unpublished. Furthermore, descriptive data and sample sizes are reported. The dataset provides a qualitative overview of the published and unpublished findings in implicit Theory of Mind custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_CsuMkuND1e_34: ent replications and non-replications", "abstract": "The current dataset contains a qualitative summary of (non-)replication studies of implicit Theory of Mind paradigms. It summarizes for each paradigm, how many replications, partial replications and non custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_CsuMkuND1e_33: "family": "Kulke", "given": "Louisa", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Rakoczy", "given": "Hannes", "parse-names": false}], "title": "Implicit Theory of Mind \u2013 An overview of curr custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_CsuMkuND1e_32: ata": {"issued": {"date-parts": [["2018"]]}, "type": "article-journal", "volume": "16", "DOI": "10.1016/j.dib.2017.11.016", "container-title": "Data in Brief", "id": "ITEM-4", "author": [{"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_CsuMkuND1e_31: ts, who may track others' knowledge and ignorance but may not consistently represent the contents of others' beliefs.", "ISSN": "08852014"}, "uris": ["http://www.mendeley.com/documents/?uuid=1794558b-9963-3bef-aed9-b39d95a549f4"]}, {"id": "ITEM-4", "itemD custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_CsuMkuND1e_30: ch we were attempting to build. However, we also discuss the consistency of our data with other findings and hypotheses regarding early-developing theory of mind, and consider the possibility that they reflect the veridical abilities of 18-month-old infan custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_zO6EBV0x8v_10: e between the ages of 3 and 4 years. Children anticipated correctly only by the age of 4 years, and performed at chance at the ages of 2 and 3 years. Moreover, we found correct anticipation only when the agent falsely believed an object to be in its last custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_wd1ieKMM68_10: uffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Kulke", "given": "Louisa", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Rakoczy", "given": "Hannes", "parse-names": fals custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_zO6EBV0x8v_11: rather than a previous location. These findings point towards the fragility of early belief-related action anticipation before the age of 4 years, when children start passing traditional false belief tasks.", "ISSN": "08852014"}, "uris": ["http://www.mend custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_wd1ieKMM68_11: e}], "title": "Implicit Theory of Mind \u2013 An overview of current replications and non-replications", "abstract": "The current dataset contains a qualitative summary of (non-)replication studies of implicit Theory of Mind paradigms. It summarizes for e custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_zO6EBV0x8v_12: eley.com/documents/?uuid=3cb02e59-0d54-3a45-8b72-a18b3f6b3825"]}, {"id": "ITEM-2", "itemData": {"issued": {"date-parts": [["2018"]]}, "type": "article-newspaper", "DOI": "10.1016/j.cogdev.2018.01.001", "container-title": "Cognitive Development", "id": "IT custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_wd1ieKMM68_12: ach paradigm, how many replications, partial replications and non-replications were identified and how many of them were published or unpublished. Furthermore, descriptive data and sample sizes are reported. The dataset provides a qualitative overview of custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_zO6EBV0x8v_13: EM-2", "author": [{"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "D\u00f6rrenberg", "given": "Sebastian", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Rakoczy", "giv custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_zO6EBV0x8v_18: on-based tasks did not replicate previous findings, suggesting that these tasks do not reliably measure ToM. Looking time and new pupil dilation measures revealed sensitivity to belief-incongruent outcomes which interacted with the presentation order of o custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_zO6EBV0x8v_19: utcomes, indicating limited evidence for implicit ToM processes under certain conditions. There were no systematic correlations of false belief processing between the tasks, thus failing to provide convergent validity. The present results suggest that the custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_wd1ieKMM68_13: the published and unpublished findings in implicit Theory of Mind research.", "ISSN": "23523409"}, "uris": ["http://www.mendeley.com/documents/?uuid=d5e1a22c-bc5e-37bc-a8a3-172144ccc302"]}]} custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_zO6EBV0x8v_14: en": "Hannes", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Liszkowski", "given": "Ulf", "parse-names": false}], "title": "How (not) to measure infant Theory of Mind: Testing the replicability and custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_zO6EBV0x8v_15: validity of four non-verbal measures", "abstract": "A growing body of infant studies with various implicit, non-verbal measures has suggested that Theory of Mind (ToM) may emerge much earlier than previously assumed. While explicit verbal ToM findings are custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_zO6EBV0x8v_16: highly replicable and show convergent validity, systematic replication studies of infant ToM, as well as convergent validations of these measures, are still missing. Here, we report a systematic study of the replicability and convergent validity of impli custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_zO6EBV0x8v_17: cit ToM tasks using four different measures with 24-month-olds (N = 66): Anticipatory looking, looking times and pupil dilation in violation-of-expectation paradigms, and spontaneous communicative interaction. Results of anticipatory looking and interacti custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_VkfvIA8rM8_9: ipated correctly only by the age of 4 years, and performed at chance at the ages of 2 and 3 years. Moreover, we found correct anticipation only when the agent falsely believed an object to be in its last rather than a previous location. These findings poi custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_VkfvIA8rM8_5: {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Disla", "given": "D.", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Steinbeis", "given": "N.", "parse-names": false} custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_VkfvIA8rM8_6: , {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Singer", "given": "T.", "parse-names": false}], "title": "Longitudinal evidence for 4-year-olds' but not 2- and 3-year-olds' false belief-related action anticipation", "abst custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_VkfvIA8rM8_7: ract": "\u00a9 2017 The Authors. Recently, infants younger than 2 years have been shown to display correct expectations of the actions of an agent with a false belief. The developmental trajectory of these early-developing abilities and their robustness, custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_VkfvIA8rM8_8: however, remain a matter of debate. Here, we tested children longitudinally from 2 to 4 years of age with an established anticipatory looking false belief task, and found a significant developmental change between the ages of 3 and 4 years. Children antic custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_VkfvIA8rM8_1: ADDIN CSL_CITATION {"schema": "https://github.com/citation-style-language/schema/raw/master/csl-citation.json", "properties": {"noteIndex": 0}, "mendeley": {"manualFormatting": "Grosse Wiesmann et al., 2017; Powell et al., 2018)Grosse Wiesmann et al., 201 custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_VkfvIA8rM8_2: 7; Powell et al., 2018)", "formattedCitation": "(Grosse Wiesmann et al., 2017; Powell et al., 2018)", "plainTextFormattedCitation": "(Grosse Wiesmann et al., 2017; Powell et al., 2018)", "previouslyFormattedCitation": "(Grosse Wiesmann et al., 2017; Powel custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_VkfvIA8rM8_3: l et al., 2018)"}, "citationItems": [{"id": "ITEM-1", "itemData": {"issued": {"date-parts": [["2017"]]}, "type": "article-journal", "DOI": "10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.08.007", "container-title": "Cognitive Development", "id": "ITEM-1", "author": [{"suffix": "" custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_VkfvIA8rM8_4: , "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Grosse Wiesmann", "given": "C.", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Friederici", "given": "A.D.", "parse-names": false}, custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_zO6EBV0x8v_21: ["http://www.mendeley.com/documents/?uuid=39ffb97a-51ed-321c-8ec0-555a9204b896"]}, {"id": "ITEM-3", "itemData": {"issued": {"date-parts": [["2018"]]}, "type": "article", "DOI": "10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.10.004", "ISBN": "0885-2014", "container-title": "Cogni custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_zO6EBV0x8v_22: tive Development", "id": "ITEM-3", "author": [{"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Powell", "given": "Lindsey J.", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "fami custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_zO6EBV0x8v_23: ly": "Hobbs", "given": "Kathryn", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Bardis", "given": "Alexandros", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle" custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_zO6EBV0x8v_24: : "", "family": "Carey", "given": "Susan", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Saxe", "given": "Rebecca", "parse-names": false}], "title": "Replications of implicit theory of mind tasks wi meta:creation-date: 2019-10-07T08:31:00Z custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_zO6EBV0x8v_20: robustness and validity of existing implicit ToM tasks needs to be treated with more caution than previously practiced, and that not all non-verbal tasks and measures are equally suited to tap into implicit ToM processing.", "ISSN": "08852014"}, "uris": custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_zO6EBV0x8v_29: ng times were consistent with infants having no clear prediction about the agent's actions under conditions of false belief. We discuss factors that might account for our failure to reproduce the previously reported violation of expectation results on whi custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_zO6EBV0x8v_25: th varying representational demands", "abstract": "We attempted to reproduce three tests of theory of mind in infants using implicit tasks that have been previously reported in the literature. These efforts were intended as initial steps in larger project custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_zO6EBV0x8v_26: s aimed at building on past research to better understand infants' theory of mind capacities and their relationship to preschoolers' explicit theory of mind. One task fully replicated evidence of 2-year-old children's sensitivity to others' ignorance. The custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_zO6EBV0x8v_27: results of another task testing for similar capacities in 18-month-old infants also elicited behavior similar to the original findings, although in that case we only conducted one of two conditions critical for demonstrating that performance depended upo custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_zO6EBV0x8v_28: n theory of mind capacities. In contrast, our violation of expectation tasks failed to reproduce evidence that, by 18 months of age, infants form specific expectations about the actions an agent will engage in on the basis of false beliefs. Instead, looki custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_9YnSGWgWUw_6: suggest that false-belief understanding does not emerge until about 4 years of age and constitutes a major developmental milestone in social cognition. By contrast, results from nontraditional tasks, which have steadily accumulated over the past 10 years, custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_9YnSGWgWUw_7: suggest that false-belief understanding is already present in infants (under age 2 years) and toddlers (age 2\u20133 years) and thus forms an integral part of social cognition from early in life. Here we first present an overview of the findings from non custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_9YnSGWgWUw_4: pping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Baillargeon", "given": "Ren\u00e9e", "parse-names": false}], "id": "ITEM-1", "ISSN": "1879307X", "DOI": "10.1016/j.tics.2017.01.012", "page": "237-249", "publisher": "Elsevier Ltd", "issue": "4" custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_9YnSGWgWUw_5: , "container-title": "Trends in Cognitive Sciences", "title": "Early False-Belief Understanding", "abstract": "Intense controversy surrounds the question of when children first understand that others can hold false beliefs. Results from traditional tasks custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_9YnSGWgWUw_8: traditional tasks. We then return to traditional tasks and argue that processing difficulties, rather than limitations in false-belief understanding, account for young children's failure at these tasks."}, "uris": ["http://www.mendeley.com/documents/?uuid custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_9YnSGWgWUw_9: =43a02a42-a054-4f6d-94c8-921dc115588a"]}, {"id": "ITEM-2", "itemData": {"issued": {"date-parts": [["2018"]]}, "type": "article-journal", "volume": "16", "DOI": "10.1016/j.dib.2017.11.016", "container-title": "Data in Brief", "id": "ITEM-2", "author": [{"s custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_9YnSGWgWUw_2: lke & Rakoczy, 2018; Scott & Baillargeon, 2017)", "plainTextFormattedCitation": "(Kulke & Rakoczy, 2018; Scott & Baillargeon, 2017)", "previouslyFormattedCitation": "(Kulke & Rakoczy, 2018; Scott & Baillargeon, 2017)"}, "citationItems": [{"id": "ITEM-1", custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_9YnSGWgWUw_3: "itemData": {"volume": "21", "issued": {"date-parts": [["2017"]]}, "type": "article-journal", "author": [{"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Scott", "given": "Rose M.", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dro custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_9YnSGWgWUw_1: ADDIN CSL_CITATION {"schema": "https://github.com/citation-style-language/schema/raw/master/csl-citation.json", "properties": {"noteIndex": 0}, "mendeley": {"manualFormatting": "Kulke & Rakoczy, 2018; Scott & Baillargeon, 2017)", "formattedCitation": "(Ku custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_SAU9bhBkts_15: cle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Rakoczy", "given": "Hannes", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Liszkowski", "given": "Ulf", "parse-names": false}], "title": "How (not) custom:DocSecurity: 0 custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_SAU9bhBkts_14: 8.01.001", "container-title": "Cognitive Development", "id": "ITEM-2", "author": [{"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "D\u00f6rrenberg", "given": "Sebastian", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-parti custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_SAU9bhBkts_13: e belief tasks.", "ISSN": "08852014"}, "uris": ["http://www.mendeley.com/documents/?uuid=3cb02e59-0d54-3a45-8b72-a18b3f6b3825"]}, {"id": "ITEM-2", "itemData": {"issued": {"date-parts": [["2018"]]}, "type": "article-newspaper", "DOI": "10.1016/j.cogdev.201 custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_SAU9bhBkts_12: nly when the agent falsely believed an object to be in its last rather than a previous location. These findings point towards the fragility of early belief-related action anticipation before the age of 4 years, when children start passing traditional fals custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_SAU9bhBkts_11: g false belief task, and found a significant developmental change between the ages of 3 and 4 years. Children anticipated correctly only by the age of 4 years, and performed at chance at the ages of 2 and 3 years. Moreover, we found correct anticipation o custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_SAU9bhBkts_10: agent with a false belief. The developmental trajectory of these early-developing abilities and their robustness, however, remain a matter of debate. Here, we tested children longitudinally from 2 to 4 years of age with an established anticipatory lookin dc:creator: Victoria custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_SAU9bhBkts_19: ative interaction. Results of anticipatory looking and interaction-based tasks did not replicate previous findings, suggesting that these tasks do not reliably measure ToM. Looking time and new pupil dilation measures revealed sensitivity to belief-incong custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_SAU9bhBkts_18: atic study of the replicability and convergent validity of implicit ToM tasks using four different measures with 24-month-olds (N = 66): Anticipatory looking, looking times and pupil dilation in violation-of-expectation paradigms, and spontaneous communic custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_SAU9bhBkts_17: than previously assumed. While explicit verbal ToM findings are highly replicable and show convergent validity, systematic replication studies of infant ToM, as well as convergent validations of these measures, are still missing. Here, we report a system custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_SAU9bhBkts_16: to measure infant Theory of Mind: Testing the replicability and validity of four non-verbal measures", "abstract": "A growing body of infant studies with various implicit, non-verbal measures has suggested that Theory of Mind (ToM) may emerge much earlier custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_SAU9bhBkts_26: se}], "title": "Replications of implicit theory of mind tasks with varying representational demands", "abstract": "We attempted to reproduce three tests of theory of mind in infants using implicit tasks that have been previously reported in the literature custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_SAU9bhBkts_25: {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Carey", "given": "Susan", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Saxe", "given": "Rebecca", "parse-names": fal creator: Victoria custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_SAU9bhBkts_24: "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Hobbs", "given": "Kathryn", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Bardis", "given": "Alexandros", "parse-names": false}, custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_SAU9bhBkts_23: dev.2017.10.004", "ISBN": "0885-2014", "container-title": "Cognitive Development", "id": "ITEM-3", "author": [{"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Powell", "given": "Lindsey J.", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_SAU9bhBkts_22: ap into implicit ToM processing.", "ISSN": "08852014"}, "uris": ["http://www.mendeley.com/documents/?uuid=39ffb97a-51ed-321c-8ec0-555a9204b896"]}, {"id": "ITEM-3", "itemData": {"issued": {"date-parts": [["2018"]]}, "type": "article", "DOI": "10.1016/j.cog custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_SAU9bhBkts_21: rovide convergent validity. The present results suggest that the robustness and validity of existing implicit ToM tasks needs to be treated with more caution than previously practiced, and that not all non-verbal tasks and measures are equally suited to t custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_SAU9bhBkts_20: ruent outcomes which interacted with the presentation order of outcomes, indicating limited evidence for implicit ToM processes under certain conditions. There were no systematic correlations of false belief processing between the tasks, thus failing to p custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_SAU9bhBkts_29: ditions critical for demonstrating that performance depended upon theory of mind capacities. In contrast, our violation of expectation tasks failed to reproduce evidence that, by 18 months of age, infants form specific expectations about the actions an ag custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_SAU9bhBkts_28: e of 2-year-old children's sensitivity to others' ignorance. The results of another task testing for similar capacities in 18-month-old infants also elicited behavior similar to the original findings, although in that case we only conducted one of two con custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_SAU9bhBkts_27: . These efforts were intended as initial steps in larger projects aimed at building on past research to better understand infants' theory of mind capacities and their relationship to preschoolers' explicit theory of mind. One task fully replicated evidenc custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_b3XL5q0Ai8_32: that they reflect the veridical abilities of 18-month-old infants, who may track others' knowledge and ignorance but may not consistently represent the contents of others' beliefs.", "ISSN": "08852014"}, "uris": ["http://www.mendeley.com/documents/?uuid= custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_b3XL5q0Ai8_31: the previously reported violation of expectation results on which we were attempting to build. However, we also discuss the consistency of our data with other findings and hypotheses regarding early-developing theory of mind, and consider the possibility custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_b3XL5q0Ai8_34: ffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Kulke", "given": "Louisa", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Rakoczy", "given": "Hannes", "parse-names": false custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_b3XL5q0Ai8_33: 1794558b-9963-3bef-aed9-b39d95a549f4"]}, {"id": "ITEM-4", "itemData": {"issued": {"date-parts": [["2018"]]}, "type": "article-journal", "volume": "16", "DOI": "10.1016/j.dib.2017.11.016", "container-title": "Data in Brief", "id": "ITEM-4", "author": [{"su custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_b3XL5q0Ai8_36: ch paradigm, how many replications, partial replications and non-replications were identified and how many of them were published or unpublished. Furthermore, descriptive data and sample sizes are reported. The dataset provides a qualitative overview of t custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_b3XL5q0Ai8_35: }], "title": "Implicit Theory of Mind \u2013 An overview of current replications and non-replications", "abstract": "The current dataset contains a qualitative summary of (non-)replication studies of implicit Theory of Mind paradigms. It summarizes for ea custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_b3XL5q0Ai8_37: he published and unpublished findings in implicit Theory of Mind research.", "ISSN": "23523409"}, "uris": ["http://www.mendeley.com/documents/?uuid=d5e1a22c-bc5e-37bc-a8a3-172144ccc302"]}]} custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_YdsCxjIXMW_14: suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Saxe", "given": "Rebecca", "parse-names": false}], "title": "Replications of implicit theory of mind tasks with varying representational demands", "abstract": "We attempted to r custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_YdsCxjIXMW_15: eproduce three tests of theory of mind in infants using implicit tasks that have been previously reported in the literature. These efforts were intended as initial steps in larger projects aimed at building on past research to better understand infants' t custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_YdsCxjIXMW_16: heory of mind capacities and their relationship to preschoolers' explicit theory of mind. One task fully replicated evidence of 2-year-old children's sensitivity to others' ignorance. The results of another task testing for similar capacities in 18-month- custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_YdsCxjIXMW_17: old infants also elicited behavior similar to the original findings, although in that case we only conducted one of two conditions critical for demonstrating that performance depended upon theory of mind capacities. In contrast, our violation of expectati custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_YdsCxjIXMW_18: on tasks failed to reproduce evidence that, by 18 months of age, infants form specific expectations about the actions an agent will engage in on the basis of false beliefs. Instead, looking times were consistent with infants having no clear prediction abo custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_YdsCxjIXMW_19: ut the agent's actions under conditions of false belief. We discuss factors that might account for our failure to reproduce the previously reported violation of expectation results on which we were attempting to build. However, we also discuss the consist custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_9YnSGWgWUw_10: uffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Kulke", "given": "Louisa", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Rakoczy", "given": "Hannes", "parse-names": fals custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_9YnSGWgWUw_13: the published and unpublished findings in implicit Theory of Mind research.", "ISSN": "23523409"}, "uris": ["http://www.mendeley.com/documents/?uuid=d5e1a22c-bc5e-37bc-a8a3-172144ccc302"]}]} custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_9YnSGWgWUw_11: e}], "title": "Implicit Theory of Mind \u2013 An overview of current replications and non-replications", "abstract": "The current dataset contains a qualitative summary of (non-)replication studies of implicit Theory of Mind paradigms. It summarizes for e custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_b3XL5q0Ai8_30: ent will engage in on the basis of false beliefs. Instead, looking times were consistent with infants having no clear prediction about the agent's actions under conditions of false belief. We discuss factors that might account for our failure to reproduce Template: Normal.dotm custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_9YnSGWgWUw_12: ach paradigm, how many replications, partial replications and non-replications were identified and how many of them were published or unpublished. Furthermore, descriptive data and sample sizes are reported. The dataset provides a qualitative overview of custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_b3XL5q0Ai8_21: rovide convergent validity. The present results suggest that the robustness and validity of existing implicit ToM tasks needs to be treated with more caution than previously practiced, and that not all non-verbal tasks and measures are equally suited to t custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_lwrclOEh4T_21: , "ISSN": "08852014"}, "uris": ["http://www.mendeley.com/documents/?uuid=1794558b-9963-3bef-aed9-b39d95a549f4"]}]} custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_b3XL5q0Ai8_20: ruent outcomes which interacted with the presentation order of outcomes, indicating limited evidence for implicit ToM processes under certain conditions. There were no systematic correlations of false belief processing between the tasks, thus failing to p custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_b3XL5q0Ai8_23: dev.2017.10.004", "ISBN": "0885-2014", "container-title": "Cognitive Development", "id": "ITEM-3", "author": [{"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Powell", "given": "Lindsey J.", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_b3XL5q0Ai8_22: ap into implicit ToM processing.", "ISSN": "08852014"}, "uris": ["http://www.mendeley.com/documents/?uuid=39ffb97a-51ed-321c-8ec0-555a9204b896"]}, {"id": "ITEM-3", "itemData": {"issued": {"date-parts": [["2018"]]}, "type": "article", "DOI": "10.1016/j.cog custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_b3XL5q0Ai8_25: {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Carey", "given": "Susan", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Saxe", "given": "Rebecca", "parse-names": fal custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_b3XL5q0Ai8_24: "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Hobbs", "given": "Kathryn", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Bardis", "given": "Alexandros", "parse-names": false}, custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_b3XL5q0Ai8_27: . These efforts were intended as initial steps in larger projects aimed at building on past research to better understand infants' theory of mind capacities and their relationship to preschoolers' explicit theory of mind. One task fully replicated evidenc custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_b3XL5q0Ai8_26: se}], "title": "Replications of implicit theory of mind tasks with varying representational demands", "abstract": "We attempted to reproduce three tests of theory of mind in infants using implicit tasks that have been previously reported in the literature custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_b3XL5q0Ai8_29: ditions critical for demonstrating that performance depended upon theory of mind capacities. In contrast, our violation of expectation tasks failed to reproduce evidence that, by 18 months of age, infants form specific expectations about the actions an ag modified: 2021-02-16T19:59:33Z custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_b3XL5q0Ai8_28: e of 2-year-old children's sensitivity to others' ignorance. The results of another task testing for similar capacities in 18-month-old infants also elicited behavior similar to the original findings, although in that case we only conducted one of two con custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_YdsCxjIXMW_20: ency of our data with other findings and hypotheses regarding early-developing theory of mind, and consider the possibility that they reflect the veridical abilities of 18-month-old infants, who may track others' knowledge and ignorance but may not consis custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_YdsCxjIXMW_21: tently represent the contents of others' beliefs.", "ISSN": "08852014"}, "uris": ["http://www.mendeley.com/documents/?uuid=1794558b-9963-3bef-aed9-b39d95a549f4"]}]} custom:Company: BBK custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_2Se2HaoPCR_2: & Harris, 2005)", "plainTextFormattedCitation": "(Goupil, Romand-Monnier, & Kouider, 2016; Knudsen & Liszkowski, 2012; Koenig & Harris, 2005)"}, "citationItems": [{"id": "ITEM-1", "itemData": {"volume": "15", "issued": {"date-parts": [["2012"]]}, "type": custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_2Se2HaoPCR_3: "article-journal", "id": "ITEM-1", "ISSN": "1363755X", "DOI": "10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01098.x", "page": "113-122", "author": [{"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Knudsen", "given": "Birgit", "parse-names": fa custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_2Se2HaoPCR_1: ADDIN CSL_CITATION {"schema": "https://github.com/citation-style-language/schema/raw/master/csl-citation.json", "properties": {"noteIndex": 0}, "mendeley": {"formattedCitation": "(Goupil, Romand-Monnier, & Kouider, 2016; Knudsen & Liszkowski, 2012; Koenig Last-Printed: 2018-08-07T14:05:00Z extended-properties:Application: LibreOffice/6.0.6.2$MacOSX_X86_64 LibreOffice_project/0c292870b25a325b5ed35f6b45599d2ea4458e77 custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_2Se2HaoPCR_8: hen the adult had witnessed the misplacement, or when she did not intend to retrieve the misplaced object. Results suggest that preverbal infants anticipate a person's mistaken action through mental attributions of both her goal and reality representation custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_2Se2HaoPCR_9: s, and correct her proactively by spontaneously providing unsolicited information."}, "uris": ["http://www.mendeley.com/documents/?uuid=98e168ba-e634-42ce-947e-43ff448d60b8"]}, {"id": "ITEM-2", "itemData": {"volume": "76", "issued": {"date-parts": [["2005 custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_2Se2HaoPCR_6: istakes. Such anticipatory correcting reveals a social motivation for unsolicited helping. Cognitively, it requires forward inferences about others' actions through mental attributions of goal and reality representations. The current study shows that infa custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_CsuMkuND1e_7: : "Steinbeis", "given": "N.", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Singer", "given": "T.", "parse-names": false}], "title": "Longitudinal evidence for 4-year-olds' but not 2- and 3-year-old custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_2Se2HaoPCR_7: nts spontaneously intervene when an adult is mistaken about the location of an object she is about to retrieve. Infants pointed out a correct location for an adult before she was about to commit a mistake. Infants did not intervene in control conditions w custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_CsuMkuND1e_8: s' false belief-related action anticipation", "abstract": "\u00a9 2017 The Authors. Recently, infants younger than 2 years have been shown to display correct expectations of the actions of an agent with a false belief. The developmental trajectory of thes custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_2Se2HaoPCR_4: lse}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Liszkowski", "given": "Ulf", "parse-names": false}], "issue": "1", "container-title": "Developmental Science", "title": "Eighteen- and 24-month-old infants correct other custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_CsuMkuND1e_9: e early-developing abilities and their robustness, however, remain a matter of debate. Here, we tested children longitudinally from 2 to 4 years of age with an established anticipatory looking false belief task, and found a significant developmental chang custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_2Se2HaoPCR_5: s in anticipation of action mistakes", "abstract": "Much of human communication and collaboration is predicated on making predictions about others' actions. Humans frequently use predictions about others' action mistakes to correct others and spare them m custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_lwrclOEh4T_20: s regarding early-developing theory of mind, and consider the possibility that they reflect the veridical abilities of 18-month-old infants, who may track others' knowledge and ignorance but may not consistently represent the contents of others' beliefs." custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_CsuMkuND1e_3: on": "(D\u00f6rrenberg et al., 2018; Grosse Wiesmann et al., 2017; Kulke & Rakoczy, 2018; Powell et al., 2018)", "previouslyFormattedCitation": "(D\u00f6rrenberg, Rakoczy, & Liszkowski, 2018; Grosse Wiesmann, Friederici, Disla, Steinbeis, & Singer, 2017; custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_b3XL5q0Ai8_10: agent with a false belief. The developmental trajectory of these early-developing abilities and their robustness, however, remain a matter of debate. Here, we tested children longitudinally from 2 to 4 years of age with an established anticipatory lookin custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_lwrclOEh4T_10: ction anticipation before the age of 4 years, when children start passing traditional false belief tasks.", "ISSN": "08852014"}, "uris": ["http://www.mendeley.com/documents/?uuid=3cb02e59-0d54-3a45-8b72-a18b3f6b3825"]}, {"id": "ITEM-2", "itemData": {"issu custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_CsuMkuND1e_4: Kulke & Rakoczy, 2018; Powell, Hobbs, Bardis, Carey, & Saxe, 2018)"}, "citationItems": [{"id": "ITEM-1", "itemData": {"issued": {"date-parts": [["2017"]]}, "type": "article-journal", "DOI": "10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.08.007", "container-title": "Cognitive Dev custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_lwrclOEh4T_11: ed": {"date-parts": [["2018"]]}, "type": "article", "DOI": "10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.10.004", "ISBN": "0885-2014", "container-title": "Cognitive Development", "id": "ITEM-2", "author": [{"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "fa custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_CsuMkuND1e_5: elopment", "id": "ITEM-1", "author": [{"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Grosse Wiesmann", "given": "C.", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "F custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_b3XL5q0Ai8_12: nly when the agent falsely believed an object to be in its last rather than a previous location. These findings point towards the fragility of early belief-related action anticipation before the age of 4 years, when children start passing traditional fals custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_lwrclOEh4T_12: mily": "Powell", "given": "Lindsey J.", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Hobbs", "given": "Kathryn", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particl custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_CsuMkuND1e_6: riederici", "given": "A.D.", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Disla", "given": "D.", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family" custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_b3XL5q0Ai8_11: g false belief task, and found a significant developmental change between the ages of 3 and 4 years. Children anticipated correctly only by the age of 4 years, and performed at chance at the ages of 2 and 3 years. Moreover, we found correct anticipation o custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_lwrclOEh4T_13: e": "", "family": "Bardis", "given": "Alexandros", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Carey", "given": "Susan", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-droppin custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_b3XL5q0Ai8_14: 8.01.001", "container-title": "Cognitive Development", "id": "ITEM-2", "author": [{"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "D\u00f6rrenberg", "given": "Sebastian", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-parti custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_lwrclOEh4T_14: g-particle": "", "family": "Saxe", "given": "Rebecca", "parse-names": false}], "title": "Replications of implicit theory of mind tasks with varying representational demands", "abstract": "We attempted to reproduce three tests of theory of mind in infants custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_b3XL5q0Ai8_13: e belief tasks.", "ISSN": "08852014"}, "uris": ["http://www.mendeley.com/documents/?uuid=3cb02e59-0d54-3a45-8b72-a18b3f6b3825"]}, {"id": "ITEM-2", "itemData": {"issued": {"date-parts": [["2018"]]}, "type": "article-newspaper", "DOI": "10.1016/j.cogdev.201 custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_lwrclOEh4T_15: using implicit tasks that have been previously reported in the literature. These efforts were intended as initial steps in larger projects aimed at building on past research to better understand infants' theory of mind capacities and their relationship to custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_CsuMkuND1e_1: ADDIN CSL_CITATION {"schema": "https://github.com/citation-style-language/schema/raw/master/csl-citation.json", "properties": {"noteIndex": 0}, "mendeley": {"manualFormatting": "D\u00f6rrenberg, Rakoczy, & Liszkowski, 2018; Grosse Wiesmann, Friederici, Di custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_b3XL5q0Ai8_16: to measure infant Theory of Mind: Testing the replicability and validity of four non-verbal measures", "abstract": "A growing body of infant studies with various implicit, non-verbal measures has suggested that Theory of Mind (ToM) may emerge much earlier custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_lwrclOEh4T_16: preschoolers' explicit theory of mind. One task fully replicated evidence of 2-year-old children's sensitivity to others' ignorance. The results of another task testing for similar capacities in 18-month-old infants also elicited behavior similar to the custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_CsuMkuND1e_2: sla, Steinbeis, & Singer, 2017; Kulke & Rakoczy, 2018; Powell, Hobbs, Bardis, Carey, & Saxe, 2018)", "formattedCitation": "(D\u00f6rrenberg et al., 2018; Grosse Wiesmann et al., 2017; Kulke & Rakoczy, 2018; Powell et al., 2018)", "plainTextFormattedCitati custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_b3XL5q0Ai8_15: cle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Rakoczy", "given": "Hannes", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Liszkowski", "given": "Ulf", "parse-names": false}], "title": "How (not) custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_lwrclOEh4T_17: original findings, although in that case we only conducted one of two conditions critical for demonstrating that performance depended upon theory of mind capacities. In contrast, our violation of expectation tasks failed to reproduce evidence that, by 18 custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_b3XL5q0Ai8_18: atic study of the replicability and convergent validity of implicit ToM tasks using four different measures with 24-month-olds (N = 66): Anticipatory looking, looking times and pupil dilation in violation-of-expectation paradigms, and spontaneous communic custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_lwrclOEh4T_18: months of age, infants form specific expectations about the actions an agent will engage in on the basis of false beliefs. Instead, looking times were consistent with infants having no clear prediction about the agent's actions under conditions of false b custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_b3XL5q0Ai8_17: than previously assumed. While explicit verbal ToM findings are highly replicable and show convergent validity, systematic replication studies of infant ToM, as well as convergent validations of these measures, are still missing. Here, we report a system custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_lwrclOEh4T_19: elief. We discuss factors that might account for our failure to reproduce the previously reported violation of expectation results on which we were attempting to build. However, we also discuss the consistency of our data with other findings and hypothese custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_b3XL5q0Ai8_19: ative interaction. Results of anticipatory looking and interaction-based tasks did not replicate previous findings, suggesting that these tasks do not reliably measure ToM. Looking time and new pupil dilation measures revealed sensitivity to belief-incong custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_YdsCxjIXMW_10: nt towards the fragility of early belief-related action anticipation before the age of 4 years, when children start passing traditional false belief tasks.", "ISSN": "08852014"}, "uris": ["http://www.mendeley.com/documents/?uuid=3cb02e59-0d54-3a45-8b72-a1 custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_YdsCxjIXMW_11: 8b3f6b3825"]}, {"id": "ITEM-2", "itemData": {"issued": {"date-parts": [["2018"]]}, "type": "article", "DOI": "10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.10.004", "ISBN": "0885-2014", "container-title": "Cognitive Development", "id": "ITEM-2", "author": [{"suffix": "", "droppi custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_YdsCxjIXMW_12: ng-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Powell", "given": "Lindsey J.", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Hobbs", "given": "Kathryn", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_YdsCxjIXMW_13: "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Bardis", "given": "Alexandros", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Carey", "given": "Susan", "parse-names": false}, {" custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_SAU9bhBkts_6: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.08.007", "container-title": "Cognitive Development", "id": "ITEM-1", "author": [{"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Grosse Wiesmann", "given": "C.", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "d custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_zO6EBV0x8v_32: ata": {"issued": {"date-parts": [["2018"]]}, "type": "article-journal", "volume": "16", "DOI": "10.1016/j.dib.2017.11.016", "container-title": "Data in Brief", "id": "ITEM-4", "author": [{"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_SAU9bhBkts_5: Grosse Wiesmann, Friederici, Disla, Steinbeis, & Singer, 2017; Kulke & Rakoczy, 2018; Powell, Hobbs, Bardis, Carey, & Saxe, 2018)"}, "citationItems": [{"id": "ITEM-1", "itemData": {"issued": {"date-parts": [["2017"]]}, "type": "article-journal", "DOI": " custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_zO6EBV0x8v_33: "family": "Kulke", "given": "Louisa", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Rakoczy", "given": "Hannes", "parse-names": false}], "title": "Implicit Theory of Mind \u2013 An overview of curr custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_SAU9bhBkts_4: \u00f6rrenberg, Rakoczy, & Liszkowski, 2018; Grosse Wiesmann, Friederici, Disla, Steinbeis, & Singer, 2017; Kulke & Rakoczy, 2018; Powell, Hobbs, Bardis, Carey, & Saxe, 2018)", "previouslyFormattedCitation": "(D\u00f6rrenberg, Rakoczy, & Liszkowski, 2018; custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_zO6EBV0x8v_34: ent replications and non-replications", "abstract": "The current dataset contains a qualitative summary of (non-)replication studies of implicit Theory of Mind paradigms. It summarizes for each paradigm, how many replications, partial replications and non custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_SAU9bhBkts_3: rey, & Saxe, 2018)", "formattedCitation": "(D\u00f6rrenberg, Rakoczy, & Liszkowski, 2018; Grosse Wiesmann, Friederici, Disla, Steinbeis, & Singer, 2017; Kulke & Rakoczy, 2018; Powell, Hobbs, Bardis, Carey, & Saxe, 2018)", "plainTextFormattedCitation": "(D custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_zO6EBV0x8v_35: -replications were identified and how many of them were published or unpublished. Furthermore, descriptive data and sample sizes are reported. The dataset provides a qualitative overview of the published and unpublished findings in implicit Theory of Mind custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_SAU9bhBkts_9: Longitudinal evidence for 4-year-olds' but not 2- and 3-year-olds' false belief-related action anticipation", "abstract": "\u00a9 2017 The Authors. Recently, infants younger than 2 years have been shown to display correct expectations of the actions of an custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_SAU9bhBkts_8: , "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Steinbeis", "given": "N.", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Singer", "given": "T.", "parse-names": false}], "title": " custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_zO6EBV0x8v_30: ch we were attempting to build. However, we also discuss the consistency of our data with other findings and hypotheses regarding early-developing theory of mind, and consider the possibility that they reflect the veridical abilities of 18-month-old infan custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_SAU9bhBkts_7: ropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Friederici", "given": "A.D.", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Disla", "given": "D.", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "" custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_zO6EBV0x8v_31: ts, who may track others' knowledge and ignorance but may not consistently represent the contents of others' beliefs.", "ISSN": "08852014"}, "uris": ["http://www.mendeley.com/documents/?uuid=1794558b-9963-3bef-aed9-b39d95a549f4"]}, {"id": "ITEM-4", "itemD custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_zO6EBV0x8v_36: research.", "ISSN": "23523409"}, "uris": ["http://www.mendeley.com/documents/?uuid=d5e1a22c-bc5e-37bc-a8a3-172144ccc302"]}]} Last-Save-Date: 2021-02-16T19:59:33Z custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_laPu57HCzJ_10: orrectly inferred that the person must have formed a false belief about the location of the objects and used this inference in resolving her referential expressions. They successfully performed retrospective revision in the opposite direction as well, cor custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_laPu57HCzJ_14: 662-9c54-ebd088627319"]}]} custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_laPu57HCzJ_13: g two different modes for tracking and updating others\u2019 mental states early in development, these results also provide clear evidence of episodic memory retrieval in young children."}, "uris": ["http://www.mendeley.com/documents/?uuid=5a180aea-268e-3 custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_laPu57HCzJ_12: sk, suggesting that they cannot retrospectively attribute beliefs or revise their initial belief attributions. However, an additional experiment provided evidence for prospective tracking of false beliefs in 18-month-olds (Experiment 2). Beyond identifyin custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_laPu57HCzJ_11: recting a mistakenly attributed false belief when this was necessary (Experiment 3). Thus, children can compute beliefs retrospectively, based on episodic memories, well before they pass explicit false-belief tasks. Eighteen-month-olds failed in such a ta custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_SAU9bhBkts_2: sla, Steinbeis, & Singer, 2017; Kulke & Rakoczy, 2018; Powell, Hobbs, Bardis, Carey, & Saxe, 2018)D\u00f6rrenberg, Rakoczy, & Liszkowski, 2018; Grosse Wiesmann, Friederici, Disla, Steinbeis, & Singer, 2017; Kulke & Rakoczy, 2018; Powell, Hobbs, Bardis, Ca custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_SAU9bhBkts_1: ADDIN CSL_CITATION {"schema": "https://github.com/citation-style-language/schema/raw/master/csl-citation.json", "properties": {"noteIndex": 0}, "mendeley": {"manualFormatting": "D\u00f6rrenberg, Rakoczy, & Liszkowski, 2018; Grosse Wiesmann, Friederici, Di custom:LinksUpToDate: false X-Parsed-By: org.apache.tika.parser.DefaultParser Total-Time: 658 custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_wd1ieKMM68_6: suggest that false-belief understanding does not emerge until about 4 years of age and constitutes a major developmental milestone in social cognition. By contrast, results from nontraditional tasks, which have steadily accumulated over the past 10 years, custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_wd1ieKMM68_7: suggest that false-belief understanding is already present in infants (under age 2 years) and toddlers (age 2\u20133 years) and thus forms an integral part of social cognition from early in life. Here we first present an overview of the findings from non custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_wd1ieKMM68_8: traditional tasks. We then return to traditional tasks and argue that processing difficulties, rather than limitations in false-belief understanding, account for young children's failure at these tasks."}, "uris": ["http://www.mendeley.com/documents/?uuid custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_wd1ieKMM68_9: =43a02a42-a054-4f6d-94c8-921dc115588a"]}, {"id": "ITEM-2", "itemData": {"issued": {"date-parts": [["2018"]]}, "type": "article-journal", "volume": "16", "DOI": "10.1016/j.dib.2017.11.016", "container-title": "Data in Brief", "id": "ITEM-2", "author": [{"s custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_zO6EBV0x8v_2: sla, Steinbeis, & Singer, 2017; Kulke & Rakoczy, 2018; Powell, Hobbs, Bardis, Carey, & Saxe, 2018)", "formattedCitation": "(D\u00f6rrenberg et al., 2018; Grosse Wiesmann et al., 2017; Kulke & Rakoczy, 2018; Powell et al., 2018)", "plainTextFormattedCitati custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_zO6EBV0x8v_1: ADDIN CSL_CITATION {"schema": "https://github.com/citation-style-language/schema/raw/master/csl-citation.json", "properties": {"noteIndex": 0}, "mendeley": {"manualFormatting": "D\u00f6rrenberg, Rakoczy, & Liszkowski, 2018; Grosse Wiesmann, Friederici, Di custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_zO6EBV0x8v_4: Kulke & Rakoczy, 2018; Powell, Hobbs, Bardis, Carey, & Saxe, 2018)"}, "citationItems": [{"id": "ITEM-1", "itemData": {"issued": {"date-parts": [["2017"]]}, "type": "article-journal", "DOI": "10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.08.007", "container-title": "Cognitive Dev custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_zO6EBV0x8v_3: on": "(D\u00f6rrenberg et al., 2018; Grosse Wiesmann et al., 2017; Kulke & Rakoczy, 2018; Powell et al., 2018)", "previouslyFormattedCitation": "(D\u00f6rrenberg, Rakoczy, & Liszkowski, 2018; Grosse Wiesmann, Friederici, Disla, Steinbeis, & Singer, 2017; custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_zO6EBV0x8v_9: e early-developing abilities and their robustness, however, remain a matter of debate. Here, we tested children longitudinally from 2 to 4 years of age with an established anticipatory looking false belief task, and found a significant developmental chang Paragraph-Count: 124 custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_zO6EBV0x8v_6: riederici", "given": "A.D.", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Disla", "given": "D.", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family" custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_zO6EBV0x8v_5: elopment", "id": "ITEM-1", "author": [{"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Grosse Wiesmann", "given": "C.", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "F custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_zO6EBV0x8v_8: s' false belief-related action anticipation", "abstract": "\u00a9 2017 The Authors. Recently, infants younger than 2 years have been shown to display correct expectations of the actions of an agent with a false belief. The developmental trajectory of thes custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_zO6EBV0x8v_7: : "Steinbeis", "given": "N.", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Singer", "given": "T.", "parse-names": false}], "title": "Longitudinal evidence for 4-year-olds' but not 2- and 3-year-old Creation-Date: 2019-10-07T08:31:00Z Character-Count-With-Spaces: 63095 Last-Author: Charlotte GW custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_IyzkJ0gX3r_2: & Baillargeon, 2017)", "formattedCitation": "(Kulke & Rakoczy, 2018; Scott & Baillargeon, 2017)", "plainTextFormattedCitation": "(Kulke & Rakoczy, 2018; Scott & Baillargeon, 2017)", "previouslyFormattedCitation": "(Kulke & Rakoczy, 2018; Scott & Baillarg custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_IyzkJ0gX3r_3: eon, 2017)"}, "citationItems": [{"id": "ITEM-1", "itemData": {"volume": "21", "issued": {"date-parts": [["2017"]]}, "type": "article-journal", "author": [{"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Scott", "given": "Ros custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_IyzkJ0gX3r_1: ADDIN CSL_CITATION {"schema": "https://github.com/citation-style-language/schema/raw/master/csl-citation.json", "properties": {"noteIndex": 0}, "mendeley": {"manualFormatting": "Kulke & Rakoczy, 2018; Scott & Baillargeon, 2017)Kulke & Rakoczy, 2018; Scott custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_IyzkJ0gX3r_12: cit Theory of Mind paradigms. It summarizes for each paradigm, how many replications, partial replications and non-replications were identified and how many of them were published or unpublished. Furthermore, descriptive data and sample sizes are reported custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_IyzkJ0gX3r_11: "Rakoczy", "given": "Hannes", "parse-names": false}], "title": "Implicit Theory of Mind \u2013 An overview of current replications and non-replications", "abstract": "The current dataset contains a qualitative summary of (non-)replication studies of impli custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_IyzkJ0gX3r_8: irst present an overview of the findings from nontraditional tasks. We then return to traditional tasks and argue that processing difficulties, rather than limitations in false-belief understanding, account for young children's failure at these tasks."}, custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_IyzkJ0gX3r_13: . The dataset provides a qualitative overview of the published and unpublished findings in implicit Theory of Mind research.", "ISSN": "23523409"}, "uris": ["http://www.mendeley.com/documents/?uuid=d5e1a22c-bc5e-37bc-a8a3-172144ccc302"]}]} custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_IyzkJ0gX3r_9: "uris": ["http://www.mendeley.com/documents/?uuid=43a02a42-a054-4f6d-94c8-921dc115588a"]}, {"id": "ITEM-2", "itemData": {"issued": {"date-parts": [["2018"]]}, "type": "article-journal", "volume": "16", "DOI": "10.1016/j.dib.2017.11.016", "container-title" custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_IyzkJ0gX3r_6: ld false beliefs. Results from traditional tasks suggest that false-belief understanding does not emerge until about 4 years of age and constitutes a major developmental milestone in social cognition. By contrast, results from nontraditional tasks, which custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_IyzkJ0gX3r_7: have steadily accumulated over the past 10 years, suggest that false-belief understanding is already present in infants (under age 2 years) and toddlers (age 2\u20133 years) and thus forms an integral part of social cognition from early in life. Here we f custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_IyzkJ0gX3r_10: : "Data in Brief", "id": "ITEM-2", "author": [{"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Kulke", "given": "Louisa", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_IyzkJ0gX3r_4: e M.", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Baillargeon", "given": "Ren\u00e9e", "parse-names": false}], "id": "ITEM-1", "ISSN": "1879307X", "DOI": "10.1016/j.tics.2017.01.012", "page": "23 custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_IyzkJ0gX3r_5: 7-249", "publisher": "Elsevier Ltd", "issue": "4", "container-title": "Trends in Cognitive Sciences", "title": "Early False-Belief Understanding", "abstract": "Intense controversy surrounds the question of when children first understand that others can ho custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_2Se2HaoPCR_21: wn whether this capacity is present early in development, or whether it emerges later with the ability to verbally report one\u2019s own mental states. Here, using a nonverbal memory-monitoring paradigm, we show that 20-month- olds can monitor and report custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_2Se2HaoPCR_20: g strategies depending on the state of their knowledge. Although it has been argued that other animals share these metacognitive abilities, only humans seem to possess the ability to explicitly communicate their own uncertainty to others. It remains unkno custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_2Se2HaoPCR_25: /www.mendeley.com/documents/?uuid=9ead398e-4f78-4dbc-a872-715cae9264b9"]}]} custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_2Se2HaoPCR_24: as used selectively to avoid making errors and to decline difficult choices. These results demonstrate that infants are able to successfully monitor their own uncertainty and share this informa- tion with others to fulfill their goals."}, "uris": ["http:/ custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_2Se2HaoPCR_23: ey had forgotten the toy location. Compared with a control group in which infants had no other option but to decide by themselves, infants given the opportunity to ask for help used this option strategically to improve their performance. Asking for help w custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_2Se2HaoPCR_22: their own uncertainty. Infants had to remember the location of a hidden toy before pointing to indicate where they wanted to recover it. In an experimental group, infants were given the possibility to ask for help through nonverbal com- munication when th custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_2Se2HaoPCR_18: ", "family": "Romand-Monnier", "given": "Margaux", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Kouider", "given": "Sid", "parse-names": false}], "issue": "13", "container-title": "Proceedings of t custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_2Se2HaoPCR_17: 10.1073/pnas.1515129113", "page": "3492-3496", "author": [{"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Goupil", "given": "Louise", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": " custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_2Se2HaoPCR_16: ved."}, "uris": ["http://www.mendeley.com/documents/?uuid=9a184b7f-b7dd-4749-b930-5744c900f9d6"]}, {"id": "ITEM-3", "itemData": {"volume": "113", "issued": {"date-parts": [["2016"]]}, "type": "article-journal", "id": "ITEM-3", "ISSN": "10916490", "DOI": " custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_2Se2HaoPCR_15: rust when learning both verbal and nonverbal information. These experiments demonstrate that preschoolers have a key strategy for assessing the reliability of information. \u00a9 2005 by the Society for Research in Child Development, Inc. All rights reser custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_2Se2HaoPCR_19: he National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America", "title": "Infants ask for help when they know they don't know", "abstract": "Uncertainty monitoring is a core property of metacognition, allow- ing individuals to adapt their decision-makin custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_2Se2HaoPCR_10: "]]}, "type": "article-journal", "id": "ITEM-2", "ISSN": "00093920", "DOI": "10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00849.x", "page": "1261-1277", "author": [{"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Koenig", "given": "Melissa A.", custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_wd1ieKMM68_2: lke & Rakoczy, 2018; Scott & Baillargeon, 2017)", "plainTextFormattedCitation": "(Kulke & Rakoczy, 2018; Scott & Baillargeon, 2017)", "previouslyFormattedCitation": "(Kulke & Rakoczy, 2018; Scott & Baillargeon, 2017)"}, "citationItems": [{"id": "ITEM-1", custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_wd1ieKMM68_3: "itemData": {"volume": "21", "issued": {"date-parts": [["2017"]]}, "type": "article-journal", "author": [{"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Scott", "given": "Rose M.", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dro custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_wd1ieKMM68_4: pping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Baillargeon", "given": "Ren\u00e9e", "parse-names": false}], "id": "ITEM-1", "ISSN": "1879307X", "DOI": "10.1016/j.tics.2017.01.012", "page": "237-249", "publisher": "Elsevier Ltd", "issue": "4" custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_wd1ieKMM68_5: , "container-title": "Trends in Cognitive Sciences", "title": "Early False-Belief Understanding", "abstract": "Intense controversy surrounds the question of when children first understand that others can hold false beliefs. Results from traditional tasks custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_2Se2HaoPCR_14: ant would be accurate in the future, sought, and endorsed information from the accurate over the inaccurate informant. In Experiment 2, both age groups displayed trust in knowledgeable over ignorant speakers. In Experiment 3, children extended selective t custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_2Se2HaoPCR_13: ferable to information from unreliable informants. In Experiment 1, children were presented with previously accurate and inaccurate informants who presented conflicting names for novel objects. 4-year-olds - but not 3-year-olds-predicted whether an inform custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_2Se2HaoPCR_12: inaccurate speakers", "abstract": "Being able to evaluate the accuracy of an informant is essential to communication. Three experiments explored preschoolers' (N = 119) understanding that, in cases of conflict, information from reliable informants is pre custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_2Se2HaoPCR_11: "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Harris", "given": "Paul L.", "parse-names": false}], "issue": "6", "container-title": "Child Development", "title": "Preschoolers mistrust ignorant and custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_wd1ieKMM68_1: ADDIN CSL_CITATION {"schema": "https://github.com/citation-style-language/schema/raw/master/csl-citation.json", "properties": {"noteIndex": 0}, "mendeley": {"manualFormatting": "Kulke & Rakoczy, 2018; Scott & Baillargeon, 2017)", "formattedCitation": "(Ku meta:print-date: 2018-08-07T14:05:00Z custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_kok2ZwgpW1_10: ed this inference in resolving her referential expressions. They successfully performed retrospective revision in the opposite direction as well, correcting a mistakenly attributed false belief when this was necessary (Experiment 3). Thus, children can co custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_laPu57HCzJ_3: bra, & Kov\u00e1cs, 2018)"}, "citationItems": [{"id": "ITEM-1", "itemData": {"volume": "115", "issued": {"date-parts": [["2018"]]}, "author": [{"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Kir\u00e1ly", "given": "Ildik\u0 meta:character-count-with-spaces: 63095 custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_laPu57HCzJ_2: sibra, & Kov\u00e1cs, 2018)", "formattedCitation": "(Kir\u00e1ly, Ol\u00e1h, Csibra, & Kov\u00e1cs, 2018)", "plainTextFormattedCitation": "(Kir\u00e1ly, Ol\u00e1h, Csibra, & Kov\u00e1cs, 2018)", "previouslyFormattedCitation": "(Kir\u00e1ly, Ol\u00e1h, Csi custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_laPu57HCzJ_1: ADDIN CSL_CITATION {"schema": "https://github.com/citation-style-language/schema/raw/master/csl-citation.json", "properties": {"noteIndex": 0}, "mendeley": {"manualFormatting": "Kir\u00e1ly, Ol\u00e1h, Csibra, & Kov\u00e1cs, 2018)Kir\u00e1ly, Ol\u00e1h, C custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_laPu57HCzJ_7: debate in psychology and cognitive science concerns the nature of young children\u2019s ability to attribute and track others\u2019 beliefs. Beliefs can be attributed in at least two different ways: prospectively, during the observation of belief-inducing custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_laPu57HCzJ_6: 73/pnas.1803505115", "type": "article-journal", "issue": "45", "ISBN": "0027-8424", "container-title": "Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences", "title": "Retrospective attribution of false beliefs in 3-year-old children", "abstract": "A current custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_laPu57HCzJ_5: iven": "Gergely", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Kov\u00e1cs", "given": "\u00c1gnes M.", "parse-names": false}], "PMID": "30322932", "id": "ITEM-1", "ISSN": "0027-8424", "DOI": "10.10 custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_ixKVoProFG_13: . The dataset provides a qualitative overview of the published and unpublished findings in implicit Theory of Mind research.", "ISSN": "23523409"}, "uris": ["http://www.mendeley.com/documents/?uuid=d5e1a22c-bc5e-37bc-a8a3-172144ccc302"]}]} custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_laPu57HCzJ_4: 0f3", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Ol\u00e1h", "given": "Katalin", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Csibra", "g custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_ixKVoProFG_12: cit Theory of Mind paradigms. It summarizes for each paradigm, how many replications, partial replications and non-replications were identified and how many of them were published or unpublished. Furthermore, descriptive data and sample sizes are reported custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_ixKVoProFG_11: "Rakoczy", "given": "Hannes", "parse-names": false}], "title": "Implicit Theory of Mind \u2013 An overview of current replications and non-replications", "abstract": "The current dataset contains a qualitative summary of (non-)replication studies of impli custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_kok2ZwgpW1_13: ults also provide clear evidence of episodic memory retrieval in young children."}, "uris": ["http://www.mendeley.com/documents/?uuid=5a180aea-268e-3662-9c54-ebd088627319"]}]} custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_ixKVoProFG_10: : "Data in Brief", "id": "ITEM-2", "author": [{"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Kulke", "given": "Louisa", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_kok2ZwgpW1_12: ons. However, an additional experiment provided evidence for prospective tracking of false beliefs in 18-month-olds (Experiment 2). Beyond identifying two different modes for tracking and updating others\u2019 mental states early in development, these res custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_laPu57HCzJ_9: correctly infer that someone had a false belief. Eighteen- and 36-month-old children observed a displacement event, which was witnessed by a person wearing sunglasses (Experiment 1). Having later discovered that the sunglasses were opaque, 36-month-olds c custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_kok2ZwgpW1_11: mpute beliefs retrospectively, based on episodic memories, well before they pass explicit false-belief tasks. Eighteen-month-olds failed in such a task, suggesting that they cannot retrospectively attribute beliefs or revise their initial belief attributi custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_laPu57HCzJ_8: situations, and in a retrospective manner, based on episodic retrieval of the details of the events that brought about the beliefs. We developed a task in which only retrospective attribution, but not prospective belief tracking, would allow children to custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_cdcKnvkSEr_8: hen the adult had witnessed the misplacement, or when she did not intend to retrieve the misplaced object. Results suggest that preverbal infants anticipate a person's mistaken action through mental attributions of both her goal and reality representation custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_cdcKnvkSEr_7: nts spontaneously intervene when an adult is mistaken about the location of an object she is about to retrieve. Infants pointed out a correct location for an adult before she was about to commit a mistake. Infants did not intervene in control conditions w custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_cdcKnvkSEr_6: istakes. Such anticipatory correcting reveals a social motivation for unsolicited helping. Cognitively, it requires forward inferences about others' actions through mental attributions of goal and reality representations. The current study shows that infa custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_cdcKnvkSEr_5: s in anticipation of action mistakes", "abstract": "Much of human communication and collaboration is predicated on making predictions about others' actions. Humans frequently use predictions about others' action mistakes to correct others and spare them m meta:author: Victoria custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_ixKVoProFG_1: ADDIN CSL_CITATION {"schema": "https://github.com/citation-style-language/schema/raw/master/csl-citation.json", "properties": {"noteIndex": 0}, "mendeley": {"manualFormatting": "Kulke & Rakoczy, 2018; Scott & Baillargeon, 2017)Kulke & Rakoczy, 2018; Scott custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_cdcKnvkSEr_9: s, and correct her proactively by spontaneously providing unsolicited information."}, "uris": ["http://www.mendeley.com/documents/?uuid=98e168ba-e634-42ce-947e-43ff448d60b8"]}, {"id": "ITEM-2", "itemData": {"volume": "76", "issued": {"date-parts": [["2005 custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_ixKVoProFG_3: eon, 2017)"}, "citationItems": [{"id": "ITEM-1", "itemData": {"volume": "21", "issued": {"date-parts": [["2017"]]}, "type": "article-journal", "author": [{"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Scott", "given": "Ros custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_ixKVoProFG_2: & Baillargeon, 2017)", "formattedCitation": "(Kulke & Rakoczy, 2018; Scott & Baillargeon, 2017)", "plainTextFormattedCitation": "(Kulke & Rakoczy, 2018; Scott & Baillargeon, 2017)", "previouslyFormattedCitation": "(Kulke & Rakoczy, 2018; Scott & Baillarg custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_ixKVoProFG_5: 7-249", "publisher": "Elsevier Ltd", "issue": "4", "container-title": "Trends in Cognitive Sciences", "title": "Early False-Belief Understanding", "abstract": "Intense controversy surrounds the question of when children first understand that others can ho custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_ixKVoProFG_4: e M.", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Baillargeon", "given": "Ren\u00e9e", "parse-names": false}], "id": "ITEM-1", "ISSN": "1879307X", "DOI": "10.1016/j.tics.2017.01.012", "page": "23 custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_cdcKnvkSEr_4: lse}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Liszkowski", "given": "Ulf", "parse-names": false}], "issue": "1", "container-title": "Developmental Science", "title": "Eighteen- and 24-month-old infants correct other custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_ixKVoProFG_7: have steadily accumulated over the past 10 years, suggest that false-belief understanding is already present in infants (under age 2 years) and toddlers (age 2\u20133 years) and thus forms an integral part of social cognition from early in life. Here we f custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_cdcKnvkSEr_3: "article-journal", "id": "ITEM-1", "ISSN": "1363755X", "DOI": "10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01098.x", "page": "113-122", "author": [{"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Knudsen", "given": "Birgit", "parse-names": fa custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_ixKVoProFG_6: ld false beliefs. Results from traditional tasks suggest that false-belief understanding does not emerge until about 4 years of age and constitutes a major developmental milestone in social cognition. By contrast, results from nontraditional tasks, which custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_cdcKnvkSEr_2: & Harris, 2005)", "plainTextFormattedCitation": "(Goupil, Romand-Monnier, & Kouider, 2016; Knudsen & Liszkowski, 2012; Koenig & Harris, 2005)"}, "citationItems": [{"id": "ITEM-1", "itemData": {"volume": "15", "issued": {"date-parts": [["2012"]]}, "type": custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_ixKVoProFG_9: "uris": ["http://www.mendeley.com/documents/?uuid=43a02a42-a054-4f6d-94c8-921dc115588a"]}, {"id": "ITEM-2", "itemData": {"issued": {"date-parts": [["2018"]]}, "type": "article-journal", "volume": "16", "DOI": "10.1016/j.dib.2017.11.016", "container-title" custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_cdcKnvkSEr_1: ADDIN CSL_CITATION {"schema": "https://github.com/citation-style-language/schema/raw/master/csl-citation.json", "properties": {"noteIndex": 0}, "mendeley": {"formattedCitation": "(Goupil, Romand-Monnier, & Kouider, 2016; Knudsen & Liszkowski, 2012; Koenig custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_ixKVoProFG_8: irst present an overview of the findings from nontraditional tasks. We then return to traditional tasks and argue that processing difficulties, rather than limitations in false-belief understanding, account for young children's failure at these tasks."}, custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_cdcKnvkSEr_13: ferable to information from unreliable informants. In Experiment 1, children were presented with previously accurate and inaccurate informants who presented conflicting names for novel objects. 4-year-olds - but not 3-year-olds-predicted whether an inform custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_cdcKnvkSEr_12: inaccurate speakers", "abstract": "Being able to evaluate the accuracy of an informant is essential to communication. Three experiments explored preschoolers' (N = 119) understanding that, in cases of conflict, information from reliable informants is pre custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_cdcKnvkSEr_11: "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Harris", "given": "Paul L.", "parse-names": false}], "issue": "6", "container-title": "Child Development", "title": "Preschoolers mistrust ignorant and custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_cdcKnvkSEr_10: "]]}, "type": "article-journal", "id": "ITEM-2", "ISSN": "00093920", "DOI": "10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00849.x", "page": "1261-1277", "author": [{"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Koenig", "given": "Melissa A.", custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_4L2UFA2NBE_2: rosse Wiesmann et al., 2017; Powell et al., 2018)", "plainTextFormattedCitation": "(Grosse Wiesmann et al., 2017; Powell et al., 2018)", "previouslyFormattedCitation": "(Grosse Wiesmann et al., 2017; Powell et al., 2018)"}, "citationItems": [{"id": "ITEM- custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_4L2UFA2NBE_3: 1", "itemData": {"issued": {"date-parts": [["2017"]]}, "type": "article-journal", "DOI": "10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.08.007", "container-title": "Cognitive Development", "id": "ITEM-1", "author": [{"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle" custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_4L2UFA2NBE_4: : "", "family": "Grosse Wiesmann", "given": "C.", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Friederici", "given": "A.D.", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-drop custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_cdcKnvkSEr_19: he National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America", "title": "Infants ask for help when they know they don't know", "abstract": "Uncertainty monitoring is a core property of metacognition, allow- ing individuals to adapt their decision-makin custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_jONX8Get5n_14: 662-9c54-ebd088627319"]}]} custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_4L2UFA2NBE_5: ping-particle": "", "family": "Disla", "given": "D.", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Steinbeis", "given": "N.", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dro custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_VkfvIA8rM8_20: ency of our data with other findings and hypotheses regarding early-developing theory of mind, and consider the possibility that they reflect the veridical abilities of 18-month-old infants, who may track others' knowledge and ignorance but may not consis custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_cdcKnvkSEr_18: ", "family": "Romand-Monnier", "given": "Margaux", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Kouider", "given": "Sid", "parse-names": false}], "issue": "13", "container-title": "Proceedings of t custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_cdcKnvkSEr_17: 10.1073/pnas.1515129113", "page": "3492-3496", "author": [{"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Goupil", "given": "Louise", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": " custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_cdcKnvkSEr_16: ved."}, "uris": ["http://www.mendeley.com/documents/?uuid=9a184b7f-b7dd-4749-b930-5744c900f9d6"]}, {"id": "ITEM-3", "itemData": {"volume": "113", "issued": {"date-parts": [["2016"]]}, "type": "article-journal", "id": "ITEM-3", "ISSN": "10916490", "DOI": " custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_cdcKnvkSEr_15: rust when learning both verbal and nonverbal information. These experiments demonstrate that preschoolers have a key strategy for assessing the reliability of information. \u00a9 2005 by the Society for Research in Child Development, Inc. All rights reser custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_4L2UFA2NBE_1: ADDIN CSL_CITATION {"schema": "https://github.com/citation-style-language/schema/raw/master/csl-citation.json", "properties": {"noteIndex": 0}, "mendeley": {"manualFormatting": "Grosse Wiesmann et al., 2017; Powell et al., 2018)", "formattedCitation": "(G custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_cdcKnvkSEr_14: ant would be accurate in the future, sought, and endorsed information from the accurate over the inaccurate informant. In Experiment 2, both age groups displayed trust in knowledgeable over ignorant speakers. In Experiment 3, children extended selective t extended-properties:Company: BBK custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_4L2UFA2NBE_6: pping-particle": "", "family": "Singer", "given": "T.", "parse-names": false}], "title": "Longitudinal evidence for 4-year-olds' but not 2- and 3-year-olds' false belief-related action anticipation", "abstract": "\u00a9 2017 The Authors. Recently, infants custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_4L2UFA2NBE_7: younger than 2 years have been shown to display correct expectations of the actions of an agent with a false belief. The developmental trajectory of these early-developing abilities and their robustness, however, remain a matter of debate. Here, we teste custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_4L2UFA2NBE_8: d children longitudinally from 2 to 4 years of age with an established anticipatory looking false belief task, and found a significant developmental change between the ages of 3 and 4 years. Children anticipated correctly only by the age of 4 years, and p meta:character-count: 53757 custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_4L2UFA2NBE_9: erformed at chance at the ages of 2 and 3 years. Moreover, we found correct anticipation only when the agent falsely believed an object to be in its last rather than a previous location. These findings point towards the fragility of early belief-related a custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_VkfvIA8rM8_18: on tasks failed to reproduce evidence that, by 18 months of age, infants form specific expectations about the actions an agent will engage in on the basis of false beliefs. Instead, looking times were consistent with infants having no clear prediction abo custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_VkfvIA8rM8_19: ut the agent's actions under conditions of false belief. We discuss factors that might account for our failure to reproduce the previously reported violation of expectation results on which we were attempting to build. However, we also discuss the consist custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_VkfvIA8rM8_14: suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Saxe", "given": "Rebecca", "parse-names": false}], "title": "Replications of implicit theory of mind tasks with varying representational demands", "abstract": "We attempted to r custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_cdcKnvkSEr_24: as used selectively to avoid making errors and to decline difficult choices. These results demonstrate that infants are able to successfully monitor their own uncertainty and share this informa- tion with others to fulfill their goals."}, "uris": ["http:/ custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_VkfvIA8rM8_15: eproduce three tests of theory of mind in infants using implicit tasks that have been previously reported in the literature. These efforts were intended as initial steps in larger projects aimed at building on past research to better understand infants' t custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_cdcKnvkSEr_23: ey had forgotten the toy location. Compared with a control group in which infants had no other option but to decide by themselves, infants given the opportunity to ask for help used this option strategically to improve their performance. Asking for help w custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_VkfvIA8rM8_16: heory of mind capacities and their relationship to preschoolers' explicit theory of mind. One task fully replicated evidence of 2-year-old children's sensitivity to others' ignorance. The results of another task testing for similar capacities in 18-month- custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_cdcKnvkSEr_22: their own uncertainty. Infants had to remember the location of a hidden toy before pointing to indicate where they wanted to recover it. In an experimental group, infants were given the possibility to ask for help through nonverbal com- munication when th custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_VkfvIA8rM8_17: old infants also elicited behavior similar to the original findings, although in that case we only conducted one of two conditions critical for demonstrating that performance depended upon theory of mind capacities. In contrast, our violation of expectati custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_cdcKnvkSEr_21: wn whether this capacity is present early in development, or whether it emerges later with the ability to verbally report one\u2019s own mental states. Here, using a nonverbal memory-monitoring paradigm, we show that 20-month- olds can monitor and report custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_VkfvIA8rM8_10: nt towards the fragility of early belief-related action anticipation before the age of 4 years, when children start passing traditional false belief tasks.", "ISSN": "08852014"}, "uris": ["http://www.mendeley.com/documents/?uuid=3cb02e59-0d54-3a45-8b72-a1 custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_cdcKnvkSEr_20: g strategies depending on the state of their knowledge. Although it has been argued that other animals share these metacognitive abilities, only humans seem to possess the ability to explicitly communicate their own uncertainty to others. It remains unkno custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_VkfvIA8rM8_11: 8b3f6b3825"]}, {"id": "ITEM-2", "itemData": {"issued": {"date-parts": [["2018"]]}, "type": "article", "DOI": "10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.10.004", "ISBN": "0885-2014", "container-title": "Cognitive Development", "id": "ITEM-2", "author": [{"suffix": "", "droppi custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_VkfvIA8rM8_12: ng-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Powell", "given": "Lindsey J.", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Hobbs", "given": "Kathryn", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_VkfvIA8rM8_13: "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Bardis", "given": "Alexandros", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Carey", "given": "Susan", "parse-names": false}, {" dc:language: en-US custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_cdcKnvkSEr_25: /www.mendeley.com/documents/?uuid=9ead398e-4f78-4dbc-a872-715cae9264b9"]}]} custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_YdsCxjIXMW_1: ADDIN CSL_CITATION {"schema": "https://github.com/citation-style-language/schema/raw/master/csl-citation.json", "properties": {"noteIndex": 0}, "mendeley": {"manualFormatting": "Grosse Wiesmann et al., 2017; Powell et al., 2018)Grosse Wiesmann et al., 201 custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_YdsCxjIXMW_2: 7; Powell et al., 2018)", "formattedCitation": "(Grosse Wiesmann et al., 2017; Powell et al., 2018)", "plainTextFormattedCitation": "(Grosse Wiesmann et al., 2017; Powell et al., 2018)", "previouslyFormattedCitation": "(Grosse Wiesmann et al., 2017; Powel custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_YdsCxjIXMW_3: l et al., 2018)"}, "citationItems": [{"id": "ITEM-1", "itemData": {"issued": {"date-parts": [["2017"]]}, "type": "article-journal", "DOI": "10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.08.007", "container-title": "Cognitive Development", "id": "ITEM-1", "author": [{"suffix": "" custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_YdsCxjIXMW_4: , "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Grosse Wiesmann", "given": "C.", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Friederici", "given": "A.D.", "parse-names": false}, custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_YdsCxjIXMW_9: ipated correctly only by the age of 4 years, and performed at chance at the ages of 2 and 3 years. Moreover, we found correct anticipation only when the agent falsely believed an object to be in its last rather than a previous location. These findings poi custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_VkfvIA8rM8_21: tently represent the contents of others' beliefs.", "ISSN": "08852014"}, "uris": ["http://www.mendeley.com/documents/?uuid=1794558b-9963-3bef-aed9-b39d95a549f4"]}]} custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_YdsCxjIXMW_5: {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Disla", "given": "D.", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Steinbeis", "given": "N.", "parse-names": false} custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_YdsCxjIXMW_6: , {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Singer", "given": "T.", "parse-names": false}], "title": "Longitudinal evidence for 4-year-olds' but not 2- and 3-year-olds' false belief-related action anticipation", "abst custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_YdsCxjIXMW_7: ract": "\u00a9 2017 The Authors. Recently, infants younger than 2 years have been shown to display correct expectations of the actions of an agent with a false belief. The developmental trajectory of these early-developing abilities and their robustness, custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_YdsCxjIXMW_8: however, remain a matter of debate. Here, we tested children longitudinally from 2 to 4 years of age with an established anticipatory looking false belief task, and found a significant developmental change between the ages of 3 and 4 years. Children antic Word-Count: 9510 Character Count: 53757 custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_jONX8Get5n_12: sk, suggesting that they cannot retrospectively attribute beliefs or revise their initial belief attributions. However, an additional experiment provided evidence for prospective tracking of false beliefs in 18-month-olds (Experiment 2). Beyond identifyin custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_jONX8Get5n_13: g two different modes for tracking and updating others\u2019 mental states early in development, these results also provide clear evidence of episodic memory retrieval in young children."}, "uris": ["http://www.mendeley.com/documents/?uuid=5a180aea-268e-3 custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_jONX8Get5n_10: orrectly inferred that the person must have formed a false belief about the location of the objects and used this inference in resolving her referential expressions. They successfully performed retrospective revision in the opposite direction as well, cor custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_jONX8Get5n_11: recting a mistakenly attributed false belief when this was necessary (Experiment 3). Thus, children can compute beliefs retrospectively, based on episodic memories, well before they pass explicit false-belief tasks. Eighteen-month-olds failed in such a ta custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_4L2UFA2NBE_13: e": "", "family": "Bardis", "given": "Alexandros", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Carey", "given": "Susan", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-droppin custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_4L2UFA2NBE_14: g-particle": "", "family": "Saxe", "given": "Rebecca", "parse-names": false}], "title": "Replications of implicit theory of mind tasks with varying representational demands", "abstract": "We attempted to reproduce three tests of theory of mind in infants custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_4L2UFA2NBE_15: using implicit tasks that have been previously reported in the literature. These efforts were intended as initial steps in larger projects aimed at building on past research to better understand infants' theory of mind capacities and their relationship to custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_4L2UFA2NBE_16: preschoolers' explicit theory of mind. One task fully replicated evidence of 2-year-old children's sensitivity to others' ignorance. The results of another task testing for similar capacities in 18-month-old infants also elicited behavior similar to the custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_4L2UFA2NBE_17: original findings, although in that case we only conducted one of two conditions critical for demonstrating that performance depended upon theory of mind capacities. In contrast, our violation of expectation tasks failed to reproduce evidence that, by 18 custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_4L2UFA2NBE_18: months of age, infants form specific expectations about the actions an agent will engage in on the basis of false beliefs. Instead, looking times were consistent with infants having no clear prediction about the agent's actions under conditions of false b custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_lwrclOEh4T_9: erformed at chance at the ages of 2 and 3 years. Moreover, we found correct anticipation only when the agent falsely believed an object to be in its last rather than a previous location. These findings point towards the fragility of early belief-related a custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_4L2UFA2NBE_19: elief. We discuss factors that might account for our failure to reproduce the previously reported violation of expectation results on which we were attempting to build. However, we also discuss the consistency of our data with other findings and hypothese custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_lwrclOEh4T_6: pping-particle": "", "family": "Singer", "given": "T.", "parse-names": false}], "title": "Longitudinal evidence for 4-year-olds' but not 2- and 3-year-olds' false belief-related action anticipation", "abstract": "\u00a9 2017 The Authors. Recently, infants custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_lwrclOEh4T_5: ping-particle": "", "family": "Disla", "given": "D.", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Steinbeis", "given": "N.", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dro custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_lwrclOEh4T_8: d children longitudinally from 2 to 4 years of age with an established anticipatory looking false belief task, and found a significant developmental change between the ages of 3 and 4 years. Children anticipated correctly only by the age of 4 years, and p custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_lwrclOEh4T_7: younger than 2 years have been shown to display correct expectations of the actions of an agent with a false belief. The developmental trajectory of these early-developing abilities and their robustness, however, remain a matter of debate. Here, we teste custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_lwrclOEh4T_2: rosse Wiesmann et al., 2017; Powell et al., 2018)", "plainTextFormattedCitation": "(Grosse Wiesmann et al., 2017; Powell et al., 2018)", "previouslyFormattedCitation": "(Grosse Wiesmann et al., 2017; Powell et al., 2018)"}, "citationItems": [{"id": "ITEM- custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_lwrclOEh4T_1: ADDIN CSL_CITATION {"schema": "https://github.com/citation-style-language/schema/raw/master/csl-citation.json", "properties": {"noteIndex": 0}, "mendeley": {"manualFormatting": "Grosse Wiesmann et al., 2017; Powell et al., 2018)", "formattedCitation": "(G custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_lwrclOEh4T_4: : "", "family": "Grosse Wiesmann", "given": "C.", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Friederici", "given": "A.D.", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-drop custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_lwrclOEh4T_3: 1", "itemData": {"issued": {"date-parts": [["2017"]]}, "type": "article-journal", "DOI": "10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.08.007", "container-title": "Cognitive Development", "id": "ITEM-1", "author": [{"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle" custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_4L2UFA2NBE_10: ction anticipation before the age of 4 years, when children start passing traditional false belief tasks.", "ISSN": "08852014"}, "uris": ["http://www.mendeley.com/documents/?uuid=3cb02e59-0d54-3a45-8b72-a18b3f6b3825"]}, {"id": "ITEM-2", "itemData": {"issu custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_4L2UFA2NBE_11: ed": {"date-parts": [["2018"]]}, "type": "article", "DOI": "10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.10.004", "ISBN": "0885-2014", "container-title": "Cognitive Development", "id": "ITEM-2", "author": [{"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "fa custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_4L2UFA2NBE_12: mily": "Powell", "given": "Lindsey J.", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Hobbs", "given": "Kathryn", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particl custom:Mendeley Document_1: True custom:Mendeley Unique User Id_1: 56f39a78-6a69-3c28-8d5f-8ef5c0ea64cc custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_4L2UFA2NBE_20: s regarding early-developing theory of mind, and consider the possibility that they reflect the veridical abilities of 18-month-old infants, who may track others' knowledge and ignorance but may not consistently represent the contents of others' beliefs." custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_4L2UFA2NBE_21: , "ISSN": "08852014"}, "uris": ["http://www.mendeley.com/documents/?uuid=1794558b-9963-3bef-aed9-b39d95a549f4"]}]} date: 2021-02-16T19:59:33Z custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_b3XL5q0Ai8_1: ADDIN CSL_CITATION {"schema": "https://github.com/citation-style-language/schema/raw/master/csl-citation.json", "properties": {"noteIndex": 0}, "mendeley": {"manualFormatting": "D\u00f6rrenberg, Rakoczy, & Liszkowski, 2018; Grosse Wiesmann, Friederici, Di custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_b3XL5q0Ai8_3: rey, & Saxe, 2018)", "formattedCitation": "(D\u00f6rrenberg, Rakoczy, & Liszkowski, 2018; Grosse Wiesmann, Friederici, Disla, Steinbeis, & Singer, 2017; Kulke & Rakoczy, 2018; Powell, Hobbs, Bardis, Carey, & Saxe, 2018)", "plainTextFormattedCitation": "(D custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_b3XL5q0Ai8_2: sla, Steinbeis, & Singer, 2017; Kulke & Rakoczy, 2018; Powell, Hobbs, Bardis, Carey, & Saxe, 2018)D\u00f6rrenberg, Rakoczy, & Liszkowski, 2018; Grosse Wiesmann, Friederici, Disla, Steinbeis, & Singer, 2017; Kulke & Rakoczy, 2018; Powell, Hobbs, Bardis, Ca custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_b3XL5q0Ai8_5: Grosse Wiesmann, Friederici, Disla, Steinbeis, & Singer, 2017; Kulke & Rakoczy, 2018; Powell, Hobbs, Bardis, Carey, & Saxe, 2018)"}, "citationItems": [{"id": "ITEM-1", "itemData": {"issued": {"date-parts": [["2017"]]}, "type": "article-journal", "DOI": " custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_b3XL5q0Ai8_4: \u00f6rrenberg, Rakoczy, & Liszkowski, 2018; Grosse Wiesmann, Friederici, Disla, Steinbeis, & Singer, 2017; Kulke & Rakoczy, 2018; Powell, Hobbs, Bardis, Carey, & Saxe, 2018)", "previouslyFormattedCitation": "(D\u00f6rrenberg, Rakoczy, & Liszkowski, 2018; custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_jONX8Get5n_9: correctly infer that someone had a false belief. Eighteen- and 36-month-old children observed a displacement event, which was witnessed by a person wearing sunglasses (Experiment 1). Having later discovered that the sunglasses were opaque, 36-month-olds c custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_b3XL5q0Ai8_7: ropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Friederici", "given": "A.D.", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Disla", "given": "D.", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "" custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_jONX8Get5n_8: situations, and in a retrospective manner, based on episodic retrieval of the details of the events that brought about the beliefs. We developed a task in which only retrospective attribution, but not prospective belief tracking, would allow children to custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_b3XL5q0Ai8_6: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.08.007", "container-title": "Cognitive Development", "id": "ITEM-1", "author": [{"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Grosse Wiesmann", "given": "C.", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "d custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_jONX8Get5n_7: debate in psychology and cognitive science concerns the nature of young children\u2019s ability to attribute and track others\u2019 beliefs. Beliefs can be attributed in at least two different ways: prospectively, during the observation of belief-inducing custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_b3XL5q0Ai8_9: Longitudinal evidence for 4-year-olds' but not 2- and 3-year-olds' false belief-related action anticipation", "abstract": "\u00a9 2017 The Authors. Recently, infants younger than 2 years have been shown to display correct expectations of the actions of an custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_jONX8Get5n_6: 73/pnas.1803505115", "type": "article-journal", "issue": "45", "ISBN": "0027-8424", "container-title": "Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences", "title": "Retrospective attribution of false beliefs in 3-year-old children", "abstract": "A current custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_b3XL5q0Ai8_8: , "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Steinbeis", "given": "N.", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Singer", "given": "T.", "parse-names": false}], "title": " custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_jONX8Get5n_5: iven": "Gergely", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Kov\u00e1cs", "given": "\u00c1gnes M.", "parse-names": false}], "PMID": "30322932", "id": "ITEM-1", "ISSN": "0027-8424", "DOI": "10.10 custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_jONX8Get5n_4: 0f3", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Ol\u00e1h", "given": "Katalin", "parse-names": false}, {"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Csibra", "g custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_jONX8Get5n_3: bra, & Kov\u00e1cs, 2018)"}, "citationItems": [{"id": "ITEM-1", "itemData": {"volume": "115", "issued": {"date-parts": [["2018"]]}, "author": [{"suffix": "", "dropping-particle": "", "non-dropping-particle": "", "family": "Kir\u00e1ly", "given": "Ildik\u0 custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_jONX8Get5n_2: sibra, & Kov\u00e1cs, 2018)", "formattedCitation": "(Kir\u00e1ly, Ol\u00e1h, Csibra, & Kov\u00e1cs, 2018)", "plainTextFormattedCitation": "(Kir\u00e1ly, Ol\u00e1h, Csibra, & Kov\u00e1cs, 2018)", "previouslyFormattedCitation": "(Kir\u00e1ly, Ol\u00e1h, Csi custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_jONX8Get5n_1: ADDIN CSL_CITATION {"schema": "https://github.com/citation-style-language/schema/raw/master/csl-citation.json", "properties": {"noteIndex": 0}, "mendeley": {"manualFormatting": "Kir\u00e1ly, Ol\u00e1h, Csibra, & Kov\u00e1cs, 2018)Kir\u00e1ly, Ol\u00e1h, C Page-Count: 19 publisher: BBK meta:page-count: 19 meta:word-count: 9510 meta:save-date: 2021-02-16T19:59:33Z Application-Name: LibreOffice/6.0.6.2$MacOSX_X86_64 LibreOffice_project/0c292870b25a325b5ed35f6b45599d2ea4458e77 custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_YfcrMlUt49_11: mpute beliefs retrospectively, based on episodic memories, well before they pass explicit false-belief tasks. Eighteen-month-olds failed in such a task, suggesting that they cannot retrospectively attribute beliefs or revise their initial belief attributi custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_YfcrMlUt49_12: ons. However, an additional experiment provided evidence for prospective tracking of false beliefs in 18-month-olds (Experiment 2). Beyond identifying two different modes for tracking and updating others\u2019 mental states early in development, these res custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_YfcrMlUt49_13: ults also provide clear evidence of episodic memory retrieval in young children."}, "uris": ["http://www.mendeley.com/documents/?uuid=5a180aea-268e-3662-9c54-ebd088627319"]}]} custom:Mendeley_Bookmark_YfcrMlUt49_10: ed this inference in resolving her referential expressions. They successfully performed retrospective revision in the opposite direction as well, correcting a mistakenly attributed false belief when this was necessary (Experiment 3). Thus, children can co custom:ShareDoc: false Revision-Number: 56